FOXP2

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Jump to navigation Jump to search

FOXP2
Protein FOXP2 PDB 2a07.png
Available structures
PDBHuman UniProt search: PDBe RCSB
Identifiers
AliasesFOXP2, CAGH44, SPCH1, TNRC10, forkhead box P2
External IDsOMIM: 605317 HomoloGene: 33482 GeneCards: FOXP2
Gene location (Human)
Chromosome 7 (human)
Chr.Chromosome 7 (human)[1]
Chromosome 7 (human)
Genomic location for FOXP2
Genomic location for FOXP2
Band7q31.1Start114,086,327 bp[1]
End114,693,772 bp[1]
RNA expression pattern
PBB GE FOXP2 gnf1h09377 at fs.png
More reference expression data
Orthologs
SpeciesHumanMouse
Entrez
Ensembl
UniProt
RefSeq (mRNA)

n/a

RefSeq (protein)

n/a

Location (UCSC)Chr 7: 114.09 – 114.69 Mbn/a
PubMed search[2]n/a
Wikidata
View/Edit Human

Forkhead box protein P2 (FOXP2) is a protein that, in humans, is encoded by the FOXP2 gene, also known as CAGH44, SPCH1 or TNRC10, and is required for proper development of speech and language.[3] FOXP2 is a transcription factor, meaning that it encodes for a regulatory protein. The gene is shared with many vertebrates, where it generally plays a role in communication (for instance, the development of bird song).

Initially identified as the genetic factor of speech disorder in KE family, FOXP2 is the first gene discovered associated with speech and language.[4] The gene is located on chromosome 7 (7q31, at the SPCH1 locus) and is expressed in fetal and adult brain, heart, lung and gut.[5][6] FOXP2 orthologs[7] have also been identified in other mammals for which complete genome data are available. The FOXP2 protein contains a forkhead-box DNA-binding domain, making it a member of the FOX group of transcription factors, involved in regulation of gene expression. In addition to this characteristic forkhead-box domain, the protein contains a polyglutamine tract, a zinc finger and a leucine zipper. The gene is more active in females than in males, to which could be attributed better language learning in females.[8]

In humans, mutations of FOXP2 cause a severe speech and language disorder.[3][9] Versions of FOXP2 exist in similar forms in distantly related vertebrates; functional studies of the gene in mice[10] and in songbirds[11] indicate that it is important for modulating plasticity of neural circuits.[12] Outside the brain FOXP2 has also been implicated in development of other tissues such as the lung and gut.[13]

FOXP2 is popularly dubbed the "language gene", but this is only partly correct since there are other genes involved in language development.[14] It directly regulates a number of other genes, including CNTNAP2, CTBP1, SRPX2 and SCN3A.[15][16][17]

Three amino acid substitutions distinguish the human FOXP2 protein from that found in mice, while two amino acid substitutions distinguish the human FOXP2 protein from that found in chimpanzees,[18] but only one of these changes is unique to humans.[13] Evidence from genetically manipulated mice[19] and human neuronal cell models[20] suggests that these changes affect the neural functions of FOXP2.

Discovery[edit]

FOXP2 and its gene were discovered as a result of investigations on an English family known as the KE family, half of whom (fifteen individuals across three generations) suffered from a speech and language disorder called developmental verbal dyspraxia. Their case was studied at the Institute of Child Health of University College London.[21] In 1990 Myrna Gopnik, Professor of Linguistics at McGill University, reported that the disorder-affected KE family had severe speech impediment with incomprehensible talk, largely characterized by grammatical deficits.[22] She hypothesized that the basis was not of learning or cognitive disability, but due to genetic factors affecting mainly grammatical ability.[23] (Her hypothesis led to a popularised existence of "grammar gene" and a controversial notion of grammar-specific disorder.[24][25]) In 1995, the University of Oxford and the Institute of Child Health researchers found that the disorder was purely genetic.[26] Remarkably, the inheritance of the disorder from one generation to the next was consistent with autosomal dominant inheritance, i.e., mutation of only a single gene on an autosome (non-sex chromosome) acting in a dominant fashion. This is one of the few known examples of Mendelian (monogenic) inheritance for a disorder affecting speech and language skills, which typically have a complex basis involving multiple genetic risk factors.[27]

FOXP2 gene is located on the long (q) arm of chromosome 7 at position 31.

In 1998, Oxford University geneticists Simon Fisher, Anthony Monaco, Cecilia S. L. Lai, Jane A. Hurst, and Faraneh Vargha-Khadem identified an autosomal dominant monogenic inheritance that is localized on a small region of chromosome 7 from DNA samples taken from the affected and unaffected members.[5] The chromosomal region (locus) contained 70 genes.[28] The locus was given the official name "SPCH1" (for speech-and-language-disorder-1) by the Human Genome Nomenclature committee. Mapping and sequencing of the chromosomal region was performed with the aid of bacterial artificial chromosome clones.[6] Around this time, the researchers identified an individual who was unrelated to the KE family, but had a similar type of speech and language disorder. In this case the child, known as CS, carried a chromosomal rearrangement (a translocation) in which part of chromosome 7 had become exchanged with part of chromosome 5. The site of breakage of chromosome 7 was located within the SPCH1 region.[6]

In 2001, the team identified in CS that the mutation is in the middle of a protein-coding gene.[3] Using a combination of bioinformatics and RNA analyses, they discovered that the gene codes for a novel protein belonging to the forkhead-box (FOX) group of transcription factors. As such, it was assigned with the official name of FOXP2. When the researchers sequenced the FOXP2 gene in the KE family, they found a heterozygous point mutation shared by all the affected individuals, but not in unaffected members of the family and other people.[3] This mutation is due to an amino-acid substitution that inhibits the DNA-binding domain of the FOXP2 protein.[29] Further screening of the gene identified multiple additional cases of FOXP2 disruption, including different point mutations[9] and chromosomal rearrangements,[30] providing evidence that damage to one copy of this gene is sufficient to derail speech and language development.

Function[edit]

Foxp2 is expressed in the developing cerebellum and the hindbrain of the embryonic day 13.5 mouse. Allen Brain Atlases

FOXP2 is required for proper brain and lung development. Knockout mice with only one functional copy of the FOXP2 gene have significantly reduced vocalizations as pups.[31] Knockout mice with no functional copies of FOXP2 are runted, display abnormalities in brain regions such as the Purkinje layer, and die an average of 21 days after birth from inadequate lung development.[13]

FOXP2 is expressed in many areas of the brain[18] including the basal ganglia and inferior frontal cortex where it is essential for brain maturation and speech and language development.[16]

A knockout mouse model has been used to examine FOXP2's role in brain development and how mutations in the two copies of FOXP2 affect vocalization. Mutations in one copy result in reduced speech while abnormalities in both copies cause major brain and lung developmental issues.[13]

The expression of FOXP2 is subject to post-transcriptional regulation, particularly micro RNA, which binds to multiple miRNA binding-sites in the neocortex, causing the repression of FOXP2 3’UTR.[32]

Clinical Significance[edit]

The FOXP2 gene has been implicated in several cognitive functions including; general brain development, language, and synaptic plasticity. The FOXP2 gene region acts as a transcription factor for the forkhead box P2 protein. Transcription factors affect other regions, and the forkhead box P2 protein has been suggested to also act as a transcription factor for hundreds of genes. This prolific involvement opens the possibility that the FOXP2 gene is much more extensive than originally thought.[33] Other targets of transcription have been researched without correlation to FOXP2. Specifically, FOXP2 has been investigated in correlation with autism and dyslexia, however with no mutation was discovered as the cause.[34][35] One well identified target is language.[36] Although some research disagrees with this correlation[37] the majority of research shows that a mutated FOXP2 causes the observed production deficiency.[33][36][38][34][39][40]

There is some evidence that the linguistic impairments associated with a mutation of the FOXP2 gene are not simply the result of a fundamental deficit in motor control. For examples, the impairments include difficulties in comprehension. Brain imaging of affected individuals indicates functional abnormalities in language-related cortical and basal/ganglia regions, demonstrating that the problems extend beyond the motor system.

Mutations in FOXP2 are among several (26 genes plus 2 intergenic) loci which correlate to ADHD diagnosis in adults - clinical ADHD is an umbrella label for a heterogenous group of genetic and neurological phonomena which may result from FOXP2 mutations or other causes.[41]

Language Disorder[edit]

Ptosis, unilateral eye droops, is often seen correlated with CAS motor dyspraxia.

It is theorized that the translocation of the 7q31.2 region of the FOXP2 gene causes a severe language impairment called Developmental Verbal Dyspraxia (DVD)[36] or Childhood Apraxia of Speech (CAS)[42] So far this type of mutation has only been discovered in three families across the world including the original KE family.[40] A missense mutation causing an arginine-to-histidine substitution (R553H) in the DNA-binding domain is thought to be the abnormality in KE.[43] This would cause a normally basic residue to be fairly acidic and highly reactive at the body's pH. A heterozygous nonsense mutation, R328X variant, produces a truncated protein involved in speech and language difficulties in one KE individual and two of their close family members. R553H and R328X mutations also affected nuclear localization, DNA-binding, and the transactivation (increased gene expression) properties of FOXP2. [44]

These individuals present with deletions, translocations, and missense mutations. When tasked with repetition and verb generation, these individuals with DVD/CAS had decreased activation in the putamen and Broca's area in fMRI studies. These areas are commonly known as areas of language function.[45] This is one of the primary reasons that FOXP2 is known as a language gene. They have delayed onset of speech, difficulty with articulation including, slurred speech, stuttering, and poor pronunciation, as well as dyspraxia.[40] It is believed that a major part of this speech deficit comes from an inability to coordinate the movements necessary to produce normal speech including mouth and tongue shaping.[36] Additionally, there are more general impairments with the processing of the grammatical and linguistic aspects of speech.[44] These findings suggest that the effects of FOXP2 are not limited to motor control, as they include comprehension among other cognitive language functions. General mild motor and cognitive deficits are noted across the board.[46] Clinically these patients can also have difficulty coughing, sneezing, and/or clearing their throats.  [36]

Evolution[edit]

Human FOXP2 gene and evolutionary conservation is shown in a multiple alignment (at bottom of figure) in this image from the UCSC Genome Browser. Note that conservation tends to cluster around coding regions (exons).

The FOXP2 gene is highly conserved in mammals.[47] The human gene differs from that in non-human primates by the substitution of two amino acids, a threonine to asparagine substitution at position 303 (T303N) and an asparagine to serine substitution at position 325 (N325S).[48] In mice it differs from that of humans by three substitutions, and in zebra finch by seven amino acids.[18][49][50] One of the two amino acid differences between human and chimps also arose independently in carnivores and bats.[13][51] Similar FOXP2 proteins can be found in songbirds, fish, and reptiles such as alligators.[52][53]

DNA sampling from Homo neanderthalensis bones indicates that their FOXP2 gene is a little different, though largely similar to those of Homo sapiens (i.e. humans).[54][55]

The FOXP2 gene showed indications of recent positive selection.[47][56] Some researchers have speculated that positive selection is crucial for the evolution of language in humans.[18] Others, however, have been unable to find a clear association between species with learned vocalizations and similar mutations in FOXP2.[52][53] Recent data from a large sample of globally distributed genomes showed no evidence of positive selection, suggesting that the original signal of positive selection may be driven by sample composition.[57] Insertion of both human mutations into mice, whose version of FOXP2 otherwise differs from the human and chimpanzee versions in only one additional base pair, causes changes in vocalizations as well as other behavioral changes, such as a reduction in exploratory tendencies, and a decrease in maze learning time. A reduction in dopamine levels and changes in the morphology of certain nerve cells are also observed.[19]

However, FOXP2 is extremely diverse in echolocating bats.[58] Twenty-two sequences of non-bat eutherian mammals revealed a total number of 20 nonsynonymous mutations in contrast to half that number of bat sequences, which showed 44 nonsynonymous mutations.[51] All cetaceans share three amino acid substitutions, but no differences were found between echolocating toothed whales and non-echolocating baleen cetaceans.[51] Within bats, however, amino acid variation correlated with different echolocating types.[51]

Interactions[edit]

FOXP2 interacts with a regulatory gene CTBP1.[59] It also downregulates CNTNAP2 gene, a member of the neurexin family found in neurons. The target gene is associated with common forms of language impairment.[60] It regulates the repeat-containing protein X-linked 2 (SRPX2), which is an epilepsy and language-associated gene in humans, and sound-controlling gene in mice.[61]

Mice[edit]

In a mouse FOXP2 gene knockouts, loss of both copies of the gene caues severe motor impairment related to cerebellar abnormalities and lack of ultrasonic vocalisations normally elicited when pups are removed from their mothers.[31] These vocalizations have important communicative roles in mother-offspring interactions. Loss of one copy was associated with impairment of ultrasonic vocalisations and a modest developmental delay. Male mice on encountering female mice produce complex ultrasonic vocalisations that have characteristics of song.[62] Mice that have the R552H point mutation carried by the KE family show cerebellar reduction and abnormal synaptic plasticity in striatal and cerebellar circuits.[10]

Humanized FOXP2 mice display altered cortico-basal ganglia circuits.[19]

The human allele of the FOXP2 gene was transferred into the mouse embryos through homologous recombination was used to create humanized FOXP2 mice.  The human variant of FOXP2 also had an effect on the exploratory behavior of the mice.  In comparison with how this gene mutation functions in humans, in the humanized mouse model showed opposite effects when testing its effect on the levels of dopamine, plasticity of synapses, patterns of expression in the striatum and behavior that was exploratory in nature.[19]

When FOXP2 expression was altered in mice, it effected many different processes including the learning motor skills and the plasticity of synapses.  It makes sense for the gene to have a role in the processing of sensory inputs because of its effects in the cortical and subcortical regions of the brain such as in the olfactory bulb, auditory and visual circuits as well as in somatosensory regions of the thalamus.  Additionally, FOXP2 is found more in the sixth layer of the cortex than in the fifth so this is consistent with it having greater roles in sensory integration. FOXP2 was also found in the medial geniculate nucleus of the mouse brain which is the processing area that auditory inputs must go through in the thalamus so it was found that its mutations play a role in delaying the development of language learning.  It also makes sense for FOXP2 mutations to have roles in the learning, integrating and outputs of motor functions due to its high expression found in the Purkinje cells and cerebellar nuclei of the Cortico-cerebellar circuits.  High FOXP2 expression has also been shown in the spiny neurons which express type 1 dopamine receptors in the striatum, substantia nigra, subthalamic nucleus and ventral tegmental area.  The negative effects of the mutations of FOXP2 in these brain regions on motor abilities were shown in mice through tasks in lab studies.  When analyzing the brain circuitry in these cases, scientists found greater levels of dopamine and decreased lengths of dendrites which caused defects in long term depression which is implicated in motor function learning and maintenance.  Through EEG studies, it was also found that these mice had increased levels of activity in their striatum which contribute to these results.  There is further evidence for mutations of targets of the FOXP2 gene shown to have roles in schizophrenia, epilepsy, autism, bipolar and intellectual disabilities.  [63]

Birds[edit]

In songbirds, FOXP2 most likely regulates genes involved in neuroplasticity.[11][64] Gene knockdown of FOXP2 in Area X of the basal ganglia in songbirds results in incomplete and inaccurate song imitation.[11] Overexpression of FoxP2 was accomplished through injection of adeno-associated virus serotype 1 (AAV1) into Area X of the brain. This overexpression produced similar effects to that of knockdown; juvenile zebra finch birds were unable to accurately imitate their tutors.[65] Similarly, in adult canaries higher FOXP2 levels also correlate with song changes.[50]

Levels of FOXP2 in adult zebra finches are significantly higher when males direct their song to females than when they sing song in other contexts.[64] “Directed” singing refers to when a male is singing to a female usually for a courtship display. “Undirected” singing occurs when for example, a male sings when other males are present or is alone.[66] Studies have found that FoxP2 levels vary depending on the social context. When the birds were singing undirected song, there was a decrease of FoxP2 expression in Area X. This downregulation was not observed and FoxP2 levels remained stable in birds singing directed song.[64]

Differences between song-learning and non-song-learning birds have been shown to be caused by differences in FOXP2 gene expression, rather than differences in the amino acid sequence of the FOXP2 protein.

FOXP2 also has possible implications in the development of bat echolocation.[48][51][67]

Chimpanzees compared to humans[edit]

Chimpanzees were also found to have the FOXP2 gene, however the human version of the gene differs by a mutation change of two amino acids.[68] FOXP2 gene mutation are implicated in a developmental speech disorder that was transmitted through a family with an autosomal dominant inheritance pattern.[3] A study in Germany sequenced FOXP2’s complementary DNA in Chimps and other species to compare it with human complementary DNA in order to find the specific changes in the sequence that have evolved through natural selection.[69] FOXP2 was found to be functionally different in humans compared to chimps and this difference is said to account for the development of structures that allow the human capability of language which chimps lack. Since FOXP2 was also found to have an effect on other genes, its effects on other genes is also being studied.[70] This mutation in the FOXP2 gene present in humans was also found in Neanderthals.[71] The human version of the gene differs from the chimp in their functions and in their appearance, due to the amino acid substitution that has evolved in the version that humans carry.  Since FOXP2 is a transcription factor, it has been found to control other genes which are being studied in their functions in language development. Additionally, there were found different effects onto the targets of the FOXP2 gene in humans in how and whether there was expression of these additional genes.  In this way, it is the FOXP2 gene’s functioning that allows humans to be able to have language but does not allow chimps this same ability. Researchers deduced that this finding could be used to further study other unique human abilities such as higher up cognitive functioning that chimps are not able to exhibit.  There can also be further clinical applications in the direction of these studies in regards to illnesses that show effects on human language ability.[20]

See also[edit]

References[edit]

  1. ^ a b c GRCh38: Ensembl release 89: ENSG00000128573 - Ensembl, May 2017
  2. ^ "Human PubMed Reference:".
  3. ^ a b c d e Lai CS, Fisher SE, Hurst JA, Vargha-Khadem F, Monaco AP (October 2001). "A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder". Nature. 413 (6855): 519–23. doi:10.1038/35097076. PMID 11586359.
  4. ^ Nudel R, Newbury DF (September 2013). "FOXP2". Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. 4 (5): 547–560. doi:10.1002/wcs.1247. PMC 3992897. PMID 24765219.
  5. ^ a b Fisher SE, Vargha-Khadem F, Watkins KE, Monaco AP, Pembrey ME (February 1998). "Localisation of a gene implicated in a severe speech and language disorder". Nature Genetics. 18 (2): 168–70. doi:10.1038/ng0298-168. PMID 9462748.
  6. ^ a b c Lai CS, Fisher SE, Hurst JA, Levy ER, Hodgson S, Fox M, et al. (August 2000). "The SPCH1 region on human 7q31: genomic characterization of the critical interval and localization of translocations associated with speech and language disorder". American Journal of Human Genetics. 67 (2): 357–68. doi:10.1086/303011. PMC 1287211. PMID 10880297.
  7. ^ "OrthoMaM phylogenetic marker: FOXP2 coding sequence".
  8. ^ Pennisi E (31 October 2013). "'Language Gene' Has a Partner". Science. Retrieved 30 October 2014.
  9. ^ a b MacDermot KD, Bonora E, Sykes N, Coupe AM, Lai CS, Vernes SC, et al. (June 2005). "Identification of FOXP2 truncation as a novel cause of developmental speech and language deficits". American Journal of Human Genetics. 76 (6): 1074–80. doi:10.1086/430841. PMC 1196445. PMID 15877281.
  10. ^ a b Groszer M, Keays DA, Deacon RM, de Bono JP, Prasad-Mulcare S, Gaub S, et al. (March 2008). "Impaired synaptic plasticity and motor learning in mice with a point mutation implicated in human speech deficits". Current Biology. 18 (5): 354–62. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.01.060. PMC 2917768. PMID 18328704.
  11. ^ a b c Haesler S, Rochefort C, Georgi B, Licznerski P, Osten P, Scharff C (December 2007). "Incomplete and inaccurate vocal imitation after knockdown of FoxP2 in songbird basal ganglia nucleus Area X". PLoS Biology. 5 (12): e321. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0050321. PMC 2100148. PMID 18052609.
  12. ^ Fisher SE, Scharff C (April 2009). "FOXP2 as a molecular window into speech and language". Trends in Genetics. 25 (4): 166–77. doi:10.1016/j.tig.2009.03.002. PMID 19304338.
  13. ^ a b c d e Shu W, Lu MM, Zhang Y, Tucker PW, Zhou D, Morrisey EE (May 2007). "Foxp2 and Foxp1 cooperatively regulate lung and esophagus development". Development. 134 (10): 1991–2000. doi:10.1242/dev.02846. PMID 17428829.
  14. ^ Harpaz Y. "Language gene found". human-brain.org. Retrieved 31 October 2014.
  15. ^ Smith RS, Kenny CJ, Ganesh V, Jang A, Borges-Monroy R, Partlow JN, et al. (September 2018). "Sodium Channel SCN3A (NaV1.3) Regulation of Human Cerebral Cortical Folding and Oral Motor Development". Neuron. 99 (5): 905–913.e7. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2018.07.052. PMID 30146301.
  16. ^ a b Spiteri E, Konopka G, Coppola G, Bomar J, Oldham M, Ou J, et al. (December 2007). "Identification of the transcriptional targets of FOXP2, a gene linked to speech and language, in developing human brain". American Journal of Human Genetics. 81 (6): 1144–57. doi:10.1086/522237. PMC 2276350. PMID 17999357.
  17. ^ Vernes SC, Spiteri E, Nicod J, Groszer M, Taylor JM, Davies KE, et al. (December 2007). "High-throughput analysis of promoter occupancy reveals direct neural targets of FOXP2, a gene mutated in speech and language disorders". American Journal of Human Genetics. 81 (6): 1232–50. doi:10.1086/522238. PMC 2276341. PMID 17999362.
  18. ^ a b c d Enard W, Przeworski M, Fisher SE, Lai CS, Wiebe V, Kitano T, et al. (August 2002). "Molecular evolution of FOXP2, a gene involved in speech and language". Nature. 418 (6900): 869–72. doi:10.1038/nature01025. PMID 12192408.
  19. ^ a b c d Enard W, Gehre S, Hammerschmidt K, Hölter SM, Blass T, Somel M, et al. (May 2009). "A Humanized Version of Foxp2 Affects Cortico-Basal Ganglia Circuits in Mice". Cell. 137 (5): 961–71. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2009.03.041. PMID 19490899.
  20. ^ a b Konopka G, Bomar JM, Winden K, Coppola G, Jonsson ZO, Gao F, et al. (November 2009). "Human-specific transcriptional regulation of CNS development genes by FOXP2". Nature. 462 (7270): 213–7. doi:10.1038/nature08549. PMC 2778075. PMID 19907493. Lay summaryScience Daily.
  21. ^ Hurst JA, Baraitser M, Auger E, Graham F, Norell S (April 1990). "An extended family with a dominantly inherited speech disorder". Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 32 (4): 352–5. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.1990.tb16948.x. PMID 2332125.
  22. ^ Gopnik M (September 1990). "Genetic basis of grammar defect". Nature. 347 (6288): 26. doi:10.1038/347026a0. PMID 2395458.
  23. ^ Gopnik M (April 1990). "Feature-blind grammar and dysphagia". Nature. 344 (6268): 715. doi:10.1038/344715a0. PMID 2330028.
  24. ^ Cowie F (1999). What's Within?: Nativism Reconsidered. New York, US: Oxford University Press. pp. 290–291. ISBN 978-0-1951-5978-3.
  25. ^ Jenkins L (2000). Biolinguistics: Exploring the Biology of Language (Revised ed.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. pp. 98–99. ISBN 978-0-5210-0391-9.
  26. ^ Vargha-Khadem F, Watkins K, Alcock K, Fletcher P, Passingham R (January 1995). "Praxic and nonverbal cognitive deficits in a large family with a genetically transmitted speech and language disorder". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 92 (3): 930–3. doi:10.1073/pnas.92.3.930. PMC 42734. PMID 7846081.
  27. ^ Fisher SE, Lai CS, Monaco AP (2003). "Deciphering the genetic basis of speech and language disorders". Annual Review of Neuroscience. 26: 57–80. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.26.041002.131144. PMID 12524432.
  28. ^ "Genes that are essential for speech". The Brain from Top to Bottom. Retrieved 31 October 2014.
  29. ^ Vernes SC, Nicod J, Elahi FM, Coventry JA, Kenny N, Coupe AM, et al. (November 2006). "Functional genetic analysis of mutations implicated in a human speech and language disorder" (PDF). Human Molecular Genetics. 15 (21): 3154–67. doi:10.1093/hmg/ddl392. PMID 16984964.
  30. ^ Feuk L, Kalervo A, Lipsanen-Nyman M, Skaug J, Nakabayashi K, Finucane B, et al. (November 2006). "Absence of a paternally inherited FOXP2 gene in developmental verbal dyspraxia". American Journal of Human Genetics. 79 (5): 965–72. doi:10.1086/508902. PMC 1698557. PMID 17033973.
  31. ^ a b Shu W, Cho JY, Jiang Y, Zhang M, Weisz D, Elder GA, et al. (July 2005). "Altered ultrasonic vocalization in mice with a disruption in the Foxp2 gene". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 102 (27): 9643–8. doi:10.1073/pnas.0503739102. PMC 1160518. PMID 15983371.
  32. ^ Clovis YM, Enard W, Marinaro F, Huttner WB, De Pietri Tonelli D (September 2012). "Convergent repression of Foxp2 3'UTR by miR-9 and miR-132 in embryonic mouse neocortex: implications for radial migration of neurons". Development. 139 (18): 3332–42. doi:10.1242/dev.078063. PMID 22874921.
  33. ^ a b Reference, Genetics Home. "FOXP2 gene". Genetics Home Reference. Retrieved 26 February 2019.
  34. ^ a b Gauthier J, Joober R, Mottron L, Laurent S, Fuchs M, De Kimpe V, et al. (April 2003). "Mutation screening of FOXP2 in individuals diagnosed with autistic disorder". American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A. 118A (2): 172–5. doi:10.1002/ajmg.a.10105. PMID 12655497.
  35. ^ MacDermot KD, Bonora E, Sykes N, Coupe AM, Lai CS, Vernes SC, et al. (June 2005). "Identification of FOXP2 truncation as a novel cause of developmental speech and language deficits". American Journal of Human Genetics. 76 (6): 1074–80. doi:10.1086/430841. PMID 15877281.
  36. ^ a b c d e Reference, Genetics Home. "FOXP2-related speech and language disorder". Genetics Home Reference. Retrieved 26 February 2019.
  37. ^ Newbury DF, Bonora E, Lamb JA, Fisher SE, Lai CS, Baird G, et al. (May 2002). "FOXP2 is not a major susceptibility gene for autism or specific language impairment". American Journal of Human Genetics. 70 (5): 1318–27. doi:10.1086/339931. PMC 447606. PMID 11894222.
  38. ^ Lennon PA, Cooper ML, Peiffer DA, Gunderson KL, Patel A, Peters S, Cheung SW, Bacino CA (April 2007). "Deletion of 7q31.1 supports involvement of FOXP2 in language impairment: clinical report and review". American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A. 143A (8): 791–8. doi:10.1002/ajmg.a.31632. PMID 17330859.
  39. ^ Rossell S, Tan E, Bozaoglu K, Neill E, Sumner P, Carruthers S, Van Rheenen T, Thomas E, Gurvich C (2017). "Is language Impairment in Schizophrenia related to Language Genes?". European Neuropsychopharmacology. 27: S459–S460. doi:10.1016/j.euroneuro.2016.09.532.
  40. ^ a b c Reuter MS, Riess A, Moog U, Briggs TA, Chandler KE, Rauch A, et al. (January 2017). "FOXP2 variants in 14 individuals with developmental speech and language disorders broaden the mutational and clinical spectrum". Journal of Medical Genetics. 54 (1): 64–72. doi:10.1136/jmedgenet-2016-104094. PMID 27572252.
  41. ^ Demontis D, Walters RK, Martin J, Mattheisen M, Als TD, Agerbo E, et al. (January 2019). "Discovery of the first genome-wide significant risk loci for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder". Nature Genetics. 51 (1): 63–75. doi:10.1038/s41588-018-0269-7. PMID 30478444.
  42. ^ Morgan A, Fisher SE, Scheffer I, Hildebrand M (23 June 2016). Adam MP, Ardinger HH, Pagon RA, Wallace SE, Bean LJ, Stephens K, Amemiya A (eds.). "FOXP2-Related Speech and Language Disorders". GenReviews. Seattle (WA): University of Washington. PMID 27336128.
  43. ^ Preuss TM (June 2012). "Human brain evolution: from gene discovery to phenotype discovery". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 109 (Suppl 1): 10709–16. doi:10.1073/pnas.1201894109. PMID 22723367.
  44. ^ a b MacDermot KD, Bonora E, Sykes N, Coupe AM, Lai CS, Vernes SC, Vargha-Khadem F, McKenzie F, Smith RL, Monaco AP, Fisher SE (June 2005). "Identification of FOXP2 truncation as a novel cause of developmental speech and language deficits". American Journal of Human Genetics. 76 (6): 1074–80. doi:10.1086/430841. PMID 15877281.
  45. ^ Vargha-Khadem F, Gadian DG, Copp A, Mishkin M (February 2005). "FOXP2 and the neuroanatomy of speech and language". Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 6 (2): 131–8. doi:10.1038/nrn1605. PMID 15685218.
  46. ^ Lennon PA, Cooper ML, Peiffer DA, Gunderson KL, Patel A, Peters S, Cheung SW, Bacino CA (April 2007). "Deletion of 7q31.1 supports involvement of FOXP2 in language impairment: clinical report and review". American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A. 143A (8): 791–8. doi:10.1002/ajmg.a.31632. PMID 17330859.
  47. ^ a b Enard W, Przeworski M, Fisher SE, Lai CS, Wiebe V, Kitano T, Monaco AP, Pääbo S (August 2002). "Molecular evolution of FOXP2, a gene involved in speech and language" (PDF). Nature. 418 (6900): 869–72. doi:10.1038/nature01025. PMID 12192408. Archived from the original (PDF) on 30 August 2006.
  48. ^ a b Preuss TM (June 2012). "Human brain evolution: from gene discovery to phenotype discovery". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 109 (Suppl 1): 10709–16. doi:10.1073/pnas.1201894109. PMC 3386880. PMID 22723367.
  49. ^ Teramitsu I, Kudo LC, London SE, Geschwind DH, White SA (March 2004). "Parallel FoxP1 and FoxP2 expression in songbird and human brain predicts functional interaction". The Journal of Neuroscience. 24 (13): 3152–63. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5589-03.2004. PMID 15056695.
  50. ^ a b Haesler S, Wada K, Nshdejan A, Morrisey EE, Lints T, Jarvis ED, Scharff C (March 2004). "FoxP2 expression in avian vocal learners and non-learners". The Journal of Neuroscience. 24 (13): 3164–75. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4369-03.2004. PMID 15056696.
  51. ^ a b c d e Li G, Wang J, Rossiter SJ, Jones G, Zhang S (September 2007). Ellegren H (ed.). "Accelerated FoxP2 evolution in echolocating bats". PLOS ONE. 2 (9): e900. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000900. PMC 1976393. PMID 17878935.
  52. ^ a b Webb DM, Zhang J (2005). "FoxP2 in song-learning birds and vocal-learning mammals". The Journal of Heredity. 96 (3): 212–6. doi:10.1093/jhered/esi025. PMID 15618302.
  53. ^ a b Scharff C, Haesler S (December 2005). "An evolutionary perspective on FoxP2: strictly for the birds?". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 15 (6): 694–703. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2005.10.004. PMID 16266802.
  54. ^ Zimmer, Carl (17 March 2016). "Humans Interbred With Hominins on Multiple Occasions, Study Finds". The New York Times. Retrieved 17 March 2016.
  55. ^ Krause J, Lalueza-Fox C, Orlando L, Enard W, Green RE, Burbano HA, Hublin JJ, Hänni C, Fortea J, de la Rasilla M, Bertranpetit J, Rosas A, Pääbo S (November 2007). "The derived FOXP2 variant of modern humans was shared with Neandertals". Current Biology. 17 (21): 1908–12. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.10.008. PMID 17949978. Lay summaryThe New York Times (19 October 2007). See also Benítez-Burraco A, Longa VM, Lorenzo G, Uriagereka J (November 2008). "Also sprach Neanderthalis... Or Did She?". Biolinguistics. 2 (2): 225–232.
  56. ^ Toda M, Okubo S, Ikigai H, Suzuki T, Suzuki Y, Hara Y, Shimamura T (1992). "The protective activity of tea catechins against experimental infection by Vibrio cholerae O1". Microbiology and Immunology. 36 (9): 999–1001. doi:10.1111/j.1348-0421.1992.tb02103.x. PMID 1461156.
  57. ^ Atkinson EG, Audesse AJ, Palacios JA, Bobo DM, Webb AE, Ramachandran S, Henn BM (September 2018). "No Evidence for Recent Selection at FOXP2 among Diverse Human Populations". Cell. 174 (6): 1424–1435.e15. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2018.06.048. PMC 6128738. PMID 30078708.
  58. ^ Li G, Wang J, Rossiter SJ, Jones G, Zhang S (September 2007). "Accelerated FoxP2 evolution in echolocating bats". PLOS ONE. 2 (9): e900. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000900. PMC 1976393. PMID 17878935.
  59. ^ Li S, Weidenfeld J, Morrisey EE (January 2004). "Transcriptional and DNA binding activity of the Foxp1/2/4 family is modulated by heterotypic and homotypic protein interactions". Molecular and Cellular Biology. 24 (2): 809–22. doi:10.1128/MCB.24.2.809-822.2004. PMC 343786. PMID 14701752.
  60. ^ Vernes SC, Newbury DF, Abrahams BS, Winchester L, Nicod J, Groszer M, Alarcón M, Oliver PL, Davies KE, Geschwind DH, Monaco AP, Fisher SE (November 2008). "A functional genetic link between distinct developmental language disorders". The New England Journal of Medicine. 359 (22): 2337–45. doi:10.1056/NEJMoa0802828. PMC 2756409. PMID 18987363.
  61. ^ Sia GM, Clem RL, Huganir RL (November 2013). "The human language-associated gene SRPX2 regulates synapse formation and vocalization in mice". Science. 342 (6161): 987–91. doi:10.1126/science.1245079. PMC 3903157. PMID 24179158.
  62. ^ Holy TE, Guo Z (December 2005). "Ultrasonic songs of male mice". PLoS Biology. 3 (12): e386. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0030386. PMC 1275525. PMID 16248680.
  63. ^ French CA, Fisher SE (October 2014). "What can mice tell us about Foxp2 function?". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 28: 72–9. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2014.07.003. PMID 25048596.
  64. ^ a b c Teramitsu I, White SA (July 2006). "FoxP2 regulation during undirected singing in adult songbirds". The Journal of Neuroscience. 26 (28): 7390–4. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1662-06.2006. PMC 2683919. PMID 16837586.
  65. ^ Heston JB, White SA (February 2015). "Behavior-linked FoxP2 regulation enables zebra finch vocal learning". The Journal of Neuroscience. 35 (7): 2885–94. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3715-14.2015. PMC 4331621. PMID 25698728.
  66. ^ Jarvis ED, Scharff C, Grossman MR, Ramos JA, Nottebohm F (October 1998). "For whom the bird sings: context-dependent gene expression". Neuron. 21 (4): 775–88. doi:10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80594-2. PMID 9808464.
  67. ^ Wilbrecht L, Nottebohm F (2003). "Vocal learning in birds and humans". Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 9 (3): 135–48. doi:10.1002/mrdd.10073. PMID 12953292.
  68. ^ Smith K (11 November 2009). "Evolution of a single gene linked to language". Nature. doi:10.1038/news.2009.1079. ISSN 1744-7933.
  69. ^ Enard W, Przeworski M, Fisher SE, Lai CS, Wiebe V, Kitano T, Monaco AP, Pääbo S (August 2002). "Molecular evolution of FOXP2, a gene involved in speech and language". Nature. 418 (6900): 869–72. doi:10.1038/nature01025. PMID 12192408.
  70. ^ "Why can't chimps talk? It's more than just genes". Reuters. 11 November 2009. Retrieved 21 February 2019.
  71. ^ Krause J, Lalueza-Fox C, Orlando L, Enard W, Green RE, Burbano HA, Hublin JJ, Hänni C, Fortea J, de la Rasilla M, Bertranpetit J, Rosas A, Pääbo S (November 2007). "The derived FOXP2 variant of modern humans was shared with Neandertals". Current Biology. 17 (21): 1908–12. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.10.008. PMID 17949978.

External links[edit]